SUMMARY
Communication among neocortical areas is largely thought to be mediated by long-range synaptic interactions between cortical neurons, with the thalamus providing only an initial relay of information from the sensory periphery. Higher-order thalamic nuclei receive strong synaptic inputs from the cortex and send robust projections back to other cortical areas, providing a distinct and potentially critical route for corticocortical communication. However, the relative contributions of corticocortical and thalamocortical inputs to higher-order cortical function remain unclear. Using imaging of neurons and axon terminals in combination with optogenetic manipulations, we find that the higher-order visual thalamus of mice has a unique impact on the posterior medial visual cortex (PM). Whereas corticocortical projections from lower cortical areas convey robust visual information to PM, higher-order thalamocortical projections convey information about global arousal state. Together, these findings suggest a key role for the higher-order thalamus in providing contextual signals that may flexibly modulate cortical sensory processing.
In brief
Neske and Cardin use imaging and optogenetics to examine the impact of thalamocortical and corticocortical projections on the higher-order cortex. They show that thalamic projections from the LP to higher-order cortical area PM primarily contribute to state-dependent modulation. In contrast, projections from cortical areas V1 and LM contribute to PM visual encoding.
Graphical Abstract
INTRODUCTION
Many fundamental sensory, motor, and cognitive operations of the brain depend on intricate communication among multiple regions of the neocortex. Synaptic interactions among hierarchically organized neocortical regions generate increasingly selective neuronal responses ranging from simple, heterogeneous tuning properties in primary sensory areas to highly specialized responses in higher-order cortices.1–3 Corticocortical communication thus comprises a complex web of feedforward and feedback connections4–6 with distinct laminar termination patterns and cellular and subcellular targets.7–9
Inter-areal cortical interactions also mediate cognitive flexibility, dynamically altering the flow of information across the cortex depending on behavioral context and past experience.10,11 This extensive series of interactions may support the functional flexibility of cortical processing, but the relative contributions of long-range projections are poorly understood. Neurons in higher-level cortical areas receive robust feedforward synaptic inputs from cortical areas occupying earlier levels of the sensory processing hierarchy, but it remains unclear how higher-order sensory receptive fields arise from the integration of distinct lower-level cortical inputs.12–14
In addition to direct corticocortical innervation, higher-order thalamic projections to the cortex provide a separate pathway for corticocortical communication. Higher-order thalamic areas, including the pulvinar nucleus in the visual system, receive most of their driving synaptic input from the cortex rather than from the sensory periphery. In turn, higher-order thalamic nuclei send projections to areas spanning the cortical hierarchy. A cortical region receiving direct, monosynaptic corticocortical connections may thus also receive disynaptic input from the same cortical source via higher-order thalamic nuclei,15 providing a transthalamic corticocortical connection that mirrors the direct one.16 Thalamocortical relay cells in higher-order thalamic nuclei receive very strong synaptic input from subcortically projecting layer 5 cortical pyramidal cells,17–19 suggesting that the higher-order thalamus could serve as a potent means of transmitting sensory information between cortical regions.20–22 However, the relative impact of direct corticocortical connections and higher-order thalamic projections on postsynaptic cortical targets remains unclear.
Both spontaneous and sensory-evoked activity in primary and higher-order cortical areas is strongly modulated by fluctuations in behavioral state.23–29 Wakeful behavioral states are associated with pupil dilation, facial and body movements, locomotion, arousal, attention, and task engagement.27,28,30,31 Variation in these markers of behavioral state are linked to fluctuations in cortical activity patterns and sensory encoding, as well as the reorganization of cortical networks.23,24,29,32 Activity in primary thalamic relay nuclei is likewise sensitive to global arousal and locomotion,33–35 suggesting that some behavioral signals in lower-order cortical areas may be inherited from primary or higher-order thalamic afferents. However, partly due to the challenges of selectively manipulating activity in higher-order thalamic nuclei, it remains unclear how behavioral-state information reaches higher-order cortical areas.
Here, we investigated the impact of long-range corticocortical and higher-order thalamocortical synaptic pathways on the sensory- and state-dependent properties of neurons in the posterior medial visual cortex (PM) in awake, behaving mice. Using in vivo imaging of genetically encoded calcium indicators in neuronal cell bodies and axon terminals in layer 2/3 of PM, along with optogenetic manipulations of discrete input pathways, we find that corticocortical and higher-order thalamocortical projections make distinct contributions to higher-order cortical circuit function. Whereas suppression of corticocortical inputs to PM reduces sensory-related activity, suppression of higher-order thalamocortical inputs primarily affects representation of contextual signals related to behavioral state. Our results provide insight into the synaptic foundations of corticocortical communication and suggest a unique role for the higher-order thalamus in promoting flexible sensory processing by cortical circuits.
RESULTS
Convergence of corticocortical and higher-order thalamocortical inputs to the higher visual cortex
To examine the relative contributions of corticocortical and thalamocortical inputs to higher-order cortical areas, we focused on the mouse PM, a higher-order visual cortical area (Figure 1A). PM occupies one of the highest levels of the mouse visual system hierarchy9,36 and receives feedforward long-range projections from several other visual cortical regions.37,38 To determine the relative abundance and density of projections to PM from different presynaptic regions, we injected a retrogradely infecting CAV2 virus39 carrying Cre recombinase into PM of mice harboring a Cre-dependent red fluorophore. Cells in the higher-order visual thalamus, the lateral posterior nucleus (LP; an analog of the pulvinar nucleus), but not the neighboring first-order visual thalamus, the dorsal lateral geniculate nucleus (dLGN), were robustly retro-labeled by CAV2-Cre injection into PM (Figure 1B). Projections from primary visual cortex (V1) and lateromedial cortex (LM) constituted 72%–90% (mean: 82%, SD: 0.05%) of the feedforward corticocortical projections to PM (see STAR Methods), suggesting that projection neurons in these cortical regions may be strong drivers of PM activity (Figures 1C and 1D). We therefore selected V1→PM and LM→PM projections for further comparison with the higher-order thalamocortical pathway from the LP (i.e., LP→PM projections) (Figures 1E and 1F).
Figure 1. Long-range corticocortical and higher-order thalamocortical projections to the mouse higher-order visual area PM.
(A) Schematic of pathways originating from the higher-order visual thalamus (lateral posterior nucleus [LP]) and multiple visual cortical regions that project to the higher-order posterior medial (PM) visual cortex.
(B) Coronal sections containing thalamocortical and corticocortical projection neurons retrogradely labeled via injection of CAV2-Cre into the PM of tdTomato reporter (Ai9) mice. Cells labeled by tdTomato send projections to the PM (dLGN, dorsal lateral geniculate nucleus; RL, rostral lateral visual cortex; AL: anterior lateral visual cortex; LM, lateral medial visual cortex). Scale bar: 500 μm.
(C) Quantification of overall cell density for thalamocortical and corticocortical projection neurons sending axons to the PM. Dotted lines connect values from individual Ai9 animals (N = 10 mice).
(D) Quantification of cell counts (normalized to the histological section with the highest cell count in each animal) along the anterior-posterior axis of the brain (relative to bregma) for each thalamocortical and corticocortical projection neuron type. Numbered arrows correspond to (B1)–(B4) (N = 10 mice).
(E) Thalamocortical (LP→PM) and corticocortical (V1→PM, LM→PM) projection axons in the PM anterogradely labeled with GCaMP6s via adeno-associated virus (AAV) injection in the corresponding presynaptic regions. Scale bar: 50 μm.
(F) Laminar distribution of axonal fluorescence intensity for each projection type (N = 4 mice per projection type).
(G) Top: schematic of in vivo imaging setup. Bottom: image from video monitoring of the mouse’s facial motion and pupil size.
(H) Fluorescence traces of Ca2+ activity from individual cell body (PM) and axon (V1→PM, LM→PM, and LP→PM) regions of interest (ROIs) imaged simultaneously with behavioral-state monitoring (locomotion speed, facial motion, and pupil size) and visual stimuli presentation.
Error bars denote SEM.
Corticocortical and higher-order thalamocortical pathways convey distinct visual information
To directly compare the activity of corticocortical and thalamocortical inputs to the PM with that of PM neurons, we expressed the calcium indicator GCaMP in either corticocortical and higher-order thalamocortical axons terminating in PM or PM neurons and assessed the state-dependent and visually evoked activity of these inputs (Figures 1G, 1H, and S1). Compared to V1 neurons, PM neurons exhibited less selectivity for visual stimulus size and a higher sensitivity to coherent motion in the visual field (Figures S2 and S3), consistent with previous reports of enhanced motion processing in mouse dorsal visual stream areas.38,40
Using a range of visual stimulation regimes (see STAR Methods), we compared the visual response magnitudes and feature selectivity of PM cell bodies with those of V1→PM, LM→PM, and LP→PM long-range axons. Stimulus sets comprised drifting gratings varying in orientation, spatial and temporal frequency, and size, as well as dots with varying motion coherence (see STAR Methods). Across all stimulation regimes, V1→PM axons consistently exhibited the strongest sensory-evoked responses of all the afferent populations (Figure 2). Because afferent axons and PM cell bodies overall exhibited modest selectivity (Figure S3), evoked response amplitudes were calculated across all stimuli for each set (Figure 2). Whereas V1→PM axons and PM cell bodies were comparable in their visual response magnitudes, LM→PM and LP→PM axons exhibited significantly smaller visual responses for many visual stimuli (Figure 2). The results were largely similar when analysis was restricted to preferred stimuli (Figure S3Y–S3BB), with the exception of size tuning. All statistical results are listed in Table S1.
Figure 2. Visual responses of thalamocortical and corticocortical inputs converging in the PM.
(A) Top: ROIs of each cell type that were significantly responsive (darker colors) or non-responsive (lighter colors) to visual stimuli varying in orientation. Bottom: visual response magnitude distributions to stimuli varying in orientation for all ROIs of each cell type. Because all ROIs were poorly tuned, response magnitudes were averaged across all stimuli. (V1→PM: N = 8 animals and n = 172 ROIs; LM→PM: N = 9 animals and n = 364 ROIs; LP→PM: N = 9 animals and n = 268 ROIs; and PM: N = 9 animals and n = 579 ROIs) (PM maximum value [max. val.] = 18.0, not shown for visualization purposes).
(B) As in (A) but for stimuli varying in spatial frequency/temporal frequency/speed tuning (V1→PM: N = 10 animals and n = 183 ROIs; LM→PM: N = 9 animals and n = 348 ROIs; LP→PM: N = 10 animals and n = 303 ROIs; and PM: N = 13 animals and n = 781 ROIs) (LM→PM minimum [min.]/max. val. = −8.2/20.6).
(C) As in (A) but for stimuli varying in size (V1→PM: N = 5 animals and n = 69 ROIs; LM→PM: N = 8 animals and n = 291 ROIs; LP→PM: N = 8 animals and n = 181 ROIs; and PM: N = 6 animals and n = 228 ROIs) (LM→PM max. val. = 12.4; PM max. val. = 17.3).
(D) As in (A) but for stimuli varying in motion coherence (V1→PM: N = 7 animals and n = 200 ROIs; LM→PM: N = 7 animals and n = 301 ROIs; LP→PM: N = 10 animals and n = 376 ROIs; and PM: N = 13 animals and n = 807 ROIs) (LP→PM min. val. = −30.1; PM max. val. = 12.7).
*p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, and ***p < 0.001; semi-weighted t test; Benjamini-Hochberg correction for false discovery rate.
Corticocortical and higher-order thalamocortical projections to PM exhibited diverse visual feature selectivity. For instance, whereas PM neurons largely lacked suppression in response to increasing stimulus size, corticocortical and higher-order thalamocortical projections to PM exhibited robust surround suppression (Figures S3D, S3K, and S3S), suggesting that the lack of suppression in PM is not inherited directly from afferents but instead may arise via input integration (see also Murgas et al.41). However, the tuning properties of PM neurons were similar to those of corticocortical inputs, including V1→PM axons (Figure S3), suggesting that some visual response features may be propagated directly from presynaptic populations in upstream visual cortical areas.13
Higher-order thalamocortical pathways convey state-related information
The spontaneous and sensory-evoked activity of neurons across the entire cortex is strongly influenced by the behavioral or arousal state. Changes in wakeful behavioral states are often associated with motor actions, such as locomotion, facial, and whisker motion, and altered pupil size.25–30,32,42–44 Although ascending neuromodulatory systems play a crucial role,24 both corticocortical and thalamocortical afferents could also be key players in cortical state modulation.45–47 We therefore considered what roles the major feedforward corticocortical (V1→PM and LM→PM) and higher-order thalamocortical (LP→PM) inputs may play in conveying state-dependent activity to PM.
PM neurons exhibited stronger state-dependent modulation than V1 neurons for locomotion (Figures S4A and S4B) and for facial motion (Figures S4C–S4E) and pupil diameter (Figures S4F–S4H) in the absence of locomotion. Consistent with this observation, V1→PM axons exhibited the weakest levels of overall state-dependent modulation (Figure 3), in direct contrast with their robust modulation by visual input. State-dependent modulation related to global arousal was stronger for higher-order thalamocortical inputs from LP (LP→PM axons) than either of the corticocortical afferents (V1→PM and LM→PM axons) (Figures 3 and S3X). Together, these results suggest that whereas corticocortical axons from the V1 carry the strongest sensory-related information to PM, higher-order thalamocortical axons from LP carry the strongest global state-related information.
Figure 3. Behavioral-state-dependent modulation of thalamocortical and corticocortical inputs to the PM.
(A) Example Ca2+ signal from an LP axon terminating in the PM (cyan). The bottom traces (black) show locomotion speed, facial motion (motion energy of the whisker pad), and pupil size (a.u. [arbitrary units]). Periods of sustained low or high motor activity (locomotion or facial motion) are indicated by shaded areas, and transition points from low to high motor activity are indicated by green lines. Analyses of modulation by facial motion and pupil dilation/constriction were limited to sustained periods without locomotion. Open blue boxes indicate high facial motion periods not included in the analysis of state transitions due to overlap with locomotion.
(B) Neuronal activity (Z scored ΔF/F) aligned to locomotion onset (green dotted line) and offset (red dotted line). Baseline activity (10–15 s after locomotion offset during sustained quiescent periods) is shown to the right for comparison.
(C) Locomotion modulation indices for ROIs of each cell type (V1→PM: N = 11 animals and n = 291 ROIs; LM→PM: N = 9 animals and n = 343 ROIs; LP→PM: N = 10 animals and n = 266 ROIs; and PM: N = 8 animals and n = 622 ROIs).
(D) Cross-correlation between neuronal activity (ΔF/F) and facial motion. Facial motion is the reference signal in the cross-correlation.
(E) Neuronal activity (Z scored ΔF/F) aligned to the onset of high facial motion.
(F) Peak correlation values between neuronal activity and facial motion for each cell type (V1→PM: N = 4 animals and n = 107 ROIs; LM→PM: N = 4 animals and n = 176 ROIs; LP→PM: N = 6 animals and n = 165 ROIs; and PM: N = 5 animals and n = 454 ROIs).
(G) Cross-correlation between neuronal activity (ΔF/F) and pupil size.
(H) Neuronal activity (Z scored ΔF/F) aligned to one pupil dilation-constriction cycle (derived from the Hilbert transform of the pupil signal).
(I) Peak correlation values between neuronal activity and pupil size for each cell type (V1→PM: N = 4 animals and n = 108 ROIs; LM→PM: N = 8 animals and n = 284 ROIs; LP→PM: N = 6 animals and n = 162 ROIs; and PM: N = 5 animals and n = 353 ROIs).
*p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, and ***p < 0.001; semi-weighted t test, Benjamini-Hochberg correction for false discovery rate. Error bars denote SEM.
The higher-order thalamic nucleus LP receives convergent synaptic inputs from multiple cortical and subcortical brain regions,22 as well as from multiple neuromodulatory systems.48,49 Previous anatomical work has shown that synaptic input to PM-projecting LP neurons largely arises from corticothalamic neurons in V1.22 We therefore compared the strength of the state-dependent modulation between two classes of V1 projection neurons: (1) corticocortical V1 neurons projecting directly to PM and (2) corticothalamic V1 neurons projecting to LP (Figures S4I and S4J). The somata of these pyramidal neurons largely resided in layer 5 and lower layer 6 (Figures S4K and S4L). As a proxy for calcium activity in the deep-layer somata, we imaged the calcium activity in the layer 1 apical dendrites of these neurons (Figures S4M, S4N, and S5F).50,51 Compared to V1 corticocortical projection neurons, V1 corticothalamic neurons projecting to LP exhibited stronger modulation by behavioral state, specifically for state transitions associated with dilations in pupil diameter during behavioral quiescence, a marker of fluctuations in global arousal state (Figures S4T–S4V). V1 projection neurons may thus be a key source of the strong state-dependent activity observed in LP→PM projections.
Functional interactions between afferents and PM neurons
To examine the functional contributions of corticocortical and thalamocortical inputs to PM, we simultaneously imaged the calcium activity of PM cell bodies and the axon terminals of V1→PM, LM→PM, or LP→PM projections. To separate axons from the local neuropil, we expressed GCaMP6s in axon terminals and a soma-targeted version of GCaMP, riboGCaMP6m,52 in PM cell bodies (Figures 4A–4C). We observed very little GCaMP labeling in the axons or dendrites of PM neurons when riboGCaMP6m was expressed in these cells alone (Figure S5A). Furthermore, imaging of in vivo preparations in which only riboGCaMP6m was expressed in PM cell bodies showed a negligible neuropil signal when compared with GCaMP6s (Figures S5B–S5E). A comparison of neuropil and cell body signals suggested little cross-contamination (Figures S5F–S5H). This dual GCaMP approach allowed the simultaneous imaging of two populations from a single imaging channel.
Figure 4. Distinct functional interactions between thalamocortical and corticocortical afferents and PM cells.
(A) Schematic of method for simultaneously monitoring the neuronal activity of PM cell bodies and long-range projection axons terminating in the PM based on expression of GCaMP6s in long-range axon terminals and ribo-GCaMP6m in PM cell bodies.
(B) In vivo field of view with PM cell bodies expressing riboGCaMP6m and LM axon terminals expressing GCaMP6s.
(C) Fluorescence traces of Ca2+ activity simultaneously recorded in PM cell bodies expressing riboGCaMP6m and projection axons expressing GCaMP6s.
(D) Mean Ca2+ event rates among the different cell types. (V1→PM: N = 5 animals and n = 332 ROIs; LM→PM: N = 4 animals and n = 211 ROIs; LP→PM: N = 5 animals and n = 191 ROIs; and PM: N = 14 animals and n = 168 ROIs).
(E) Ca2+ event rates for each afferent population aligned to simultaneously recorded Ca2+ events in PM neurons (raw data minus data from shuffled PM event times).
(F) Peak Ca2+ event rates aligned to PM neuron Ca2+ events (V1→PM: N = 5 animals and n = 59 PM ROIs; LM→PM: N = 4 animals and n = 53 PM ROIs; LP→PM: N = 5 animals and n = 56 PM ROIs; and PM activity aligned with PM events: N = 14 animals and n = 168 PM ROIs).
*p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, and ***p < 0.001; semi-weighted t test; Benjamini-Hochberg correction for false discovery rate. Error bars denote SEM.
Using this approach, we examined the relationship between neuronal activity in PM cell bodies and V1→PM, LM→PM, and LP→PM axons during quiescent (non-locomotion) periods. After deconvolving ΔF/F calcium activity traces from axons and cell bodies into estimates of calcium events (STAR Methods), we assessed the relationship between simultaneously imaged axonal and PM somatic calcium event rates. Although overall calcium event rates in LP axons were significantly higher than those of V1 or LM corticocortical axons and PM cell bodies (Figure 4D), LP events were significantly less time-locked to events in PM cell bodies than those of corticocortical axons (Figures 4E and 4F). LP→PM activity may thus be less effective in driving PM neuron spiking than corticocortical inputs. Indeed, functional interactions between LP→PM axons and PM cell bodies were comparable with those between LP→V1 axons and V1 cell bodies, a canonical modulatory pathway (Figure S6).53,54 Calcium events in PM cell bodies were significantly more correlated with events in neighboring PM cell bodies than those in either corticocortical or thalamocortical axons (Figures 4E and 4F), potentially reflecting strong levels of recurrent connectivity within PM.55 Together, these results support a modulatory role for LP inputs to PM and a stronger driving role for short- and long-range corticocortical inputs.
Acute silencing reveals distinct causal contributions to PM neuronal activity
To determine more precisely the contributions of each input to the activity of PM neurons, we selectively silenced individual inputs to PM while monitoring the state-dependent and visually evoked activity of PM neurons. Using Cre-dependent expression of the inhibitory opsin eOPN3,56 we silenced the axon terminals of different corticocortical and higher-order thalamocortical projections within PM. To express eOPN3 selectively in V1→PM, LM→PM, or LP→PM terminals, we injected a mixture of CAV2-Cre and AAV-GCaMP6s into the PM followed by the Cre-dependent AAV-SIO-eOPN3-mScarlet in the presynaptic target area (Figure 5A). These injections resulted in projection-specific expression of eOPN3 in axon terminals that were physically intermingled with PM cell bodies expressing GCaMP6s (Figure 5B). The relative numbers of V1→PM, LM→PM, or LP→PM projection neurons labeled in this way largely recapitulated those seen from CAV2-Cre injection in reporter animals (Figure S7), indicating that eOPN3 was not over- or under-expressed in any projection neuron class. Visual responses of PM neurons were robustly decreased by optogenetic suppression of corticocortical V1 and LM axons but only slightly affected by suppression of thalamocortical LP axons (Figures 5C, 5D, and 5G). Consistent with the strong state-dependent modulation of LP axons we observed in PM, state-dependent modulation of PM neuron activity with changes in pupil diameter was significantly reduced when LP→PM, but not V1→PM or LM→PM, terminals were inhibited (Figures 5E, 5F, 5H, and S7D–S7I). In a separate series of experiments, we selectively ablated PM-projecting neurons in either V1 or LP (see STAR Methods). Consistent with the results of optogenetic manipulation, ablation of V1→PM, but not LP→PM, projection neurons reduced visual responses in PM neurons (Figures S7J–SJP). Corticocortical and higher-order thalamocortical inputs may thus primarily confer the sensory- and state-dependent properties of PM cortical neurons, respectively (Figure 6).
Figure 5. Distinct contributions of cortico-cortical and thalamocortical pathways to PM activity.
(A) Schematic of viral injections for co-expressing GCaMP6s in PM cortical neurons and the Cre-dependent inhibitory opsin eOPN3 in presynaptic projection axon terminals.
(B) Left: expression of eOPN3-mScarlet (red) in thalamocortical neurons in the LP. Right: expression of GCaMP6s in PM cortical neurons (green) and eOPN3-mScarlet (red) in the surrounding LP thalamocortical terminals. Scale bar: 30 μm.
(C) Visual contrast-response curves of PM neurons in control animals and animals with eOPN3 expressed in corticocortical and higher-order thalamocortical axons for sessions with (light-emitting diode [LED]) and without (dark) optogenetic stimulation.
(D) Scatterplots showing visual response magnitudes from individual PM neurons identified during both dark and LED imaging sessions.
(E) As in (C) but for cross-correlations between PM neuronal activity and pupil size.
(F) As in (D) but for peak correlation values between neuronal activity and pupil size.
(G) Differences between visual response magnitudes during sessions with and without optogenetic stimulation (control: N = 5 animals and n = 195 ROIs; V1→PM eOPN3: N = 7 animals and n = 227 ROIs; LM→PM eOPN3: N = 4 animals and n = 215 ROIs; and LP→PM eOPN3: N = 6 animals and n = 327 ROIs) (control min./max. val. = −3.2/2.5; V1→PM eOPN3 min./max. val. = −5.3/3.1; LM→PM eOPN3 min./max. val. = −3.1/5.0; and LP→PM eOPN3 min./max. val. = −3.1/1.0).
(H) As in (G) but for differences in peak ΔF/F-pupil correlation values between sessions with and without optogenetic stimulation (control: N = 7 animals and n = 327 ROIs; V1→PM eOPN3: N = 4 animals and n = 169 ROIs; LM→PM eOPN3: N = 3 animals and n = 167 ROIs; and LP→PM eOPN3: N = 5 animals and n = 468 ROIs).
*p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, and ***p < 0.001; semi-weighted t test; Benjamini-Hochberg correction for false discovery rate. Error bars denote SEM.
Figure 6. Corticocortical and thalamocortical pathways may have distinct impacts on higher-order cortex.
Top: corticocortical pathways, particularly the direct input from the V1, carry strong sensory information to the higher-order visual cortex (PM), whereas higher-order thalamocortical inputs from the LP carry comparatively weak, untuned sensory signals. Bottom: corticocortical inputs to the PM carry some behavioral state information, whereas higher-order thalamocortical inputs from the LP convey strong behavioral-state-related signals that may reflect their presynaptic corticothalamic inputs from the V1.
DISCUSSION
We examined the sensory responses and behavioral-state-dependent modulation of activity in two lower-order visual cortical areas, V1 and LM, and a higher-order thalamocortical area, LP, that converge on a common higher-order visual cortical area, PM. Overall, we find that while corticocortical projections carry strong sensory information and significantly impact the visually evoked responses of their postsynaptic targets in PM, higher-order thalamocortical signals from LP do not strongly contribute to PM visual responses. In contrast, direct cortico-cortical inputs from V1 and LM do not drive state-dependent modulation in PM, whereas inputs from LP contribute to the representation of global arousal by PM neurons, as measured by pupil diameter. Together, these results suggest two streams conveying sensory and contextual information to the higher-order cortex.
Unlike first-order thalamic nuclei, which receive their driving synaptic input from the sensory periphery or associated brainstem nuclei, most higher-order thalamic nuclei receive their strongest input from the cortex. Anatomical connectivity suggests that whereas first-order thalamic nuclei (e.g., dLGN) predominately function as sensory relays, albeit under dynamic modulatory control from the cortex,33,34,57 higher-order thalamic nuclei (e.g., pulvinar) relay the results of computations carried out within local cortical networks. Like the first-order thalamus, which encodes low-level sensory information from the sensory periphery, higher-order thalamic nuclei may encode integrated sensory information that is propagated across the cortex.58 Alternatively, higher-order thalamic nuclei may predominantly convey non-sensory contextual information or reconfigure the cortical circuits to which they project.59 Previous work has suggested that the broad tuning properties of higher-order visual thalamic outputs seem inadequately suited to generate the highly specific receptive fields of extrastriate neurons60 but may instead play an essential role in modifying the functional connectivity of distributed cortical regions in a task- or context-dependent manner.61–63
Targeted imaging of corticocortical inputs to PM and PM neurons revealed robust visual responses.22,41,64 Overall, PM neurons exhibited low selectivity for stimulus orientation, enhanced motion sensitivity, and almost no surround suppression as compared to V1 neurons. We found that PM neurons exhibited enhanced tuning for high spatial frequencies.22,64 We observed less overall selectivity of PM neurons for visual features compared to some previous reports, but these differences may reflect different inclusion criteria, calcium indicators, and use of anesthesia.64–66 Observational experiments have revealed that LP neurons exhibit visual responses, but the postsynaptic impact of this visual activity is unclear. In good agreement with previous studies,22,67 we observed that LP afferents to the PM showed visual responses but with low selectivity for stimulus orientation and little size tuning. However, LP afferents exhibited high levels of activity but little correlation with the activity of individual PM neurons, consistent with a modulatory, rather than driving, role for higher-order thalamic inputs. Indeed, suppression of LP afferents did not change PM visual responses. Thus, although individual LP axons exhibit modest visual responses, these afferents do not appear to play a critical role in the visual responses of postsynaptic PM neurons. In contrast, we found that the suppression of afferents from V1 and LM strongly reduced PM visual responses. Convergent lines of evidence from the ablation of projection neurons in LP and V1 likewise supported a role for V1, but not LP, in driving PM visual responses. Together, these results suggest that some visual information carried by afferents to PM may be redundant. Alternatively, despite exhibiting visual responses, the temporal dynamics or synaptic properties of LP→PM inputs may minimize the transmission of rapidly modulated visual activity while permitting the transmission of slow time-varying fluctuations related to behavioral context or global arousal state. Reduced visual response amplitudes could potentially result from net decreases in PM neuronal excitability due to the loss of excitatory input. However, we found that the suppression of corticocortical inputs did not affect the state-dependent modulation of PM neurons, suggesting a selective role for these projections in providing visual information to the PM.
The activity levels and functional connectivity of primary and higher-order cortical areas are modulated by both global arousal state and motor activity.23,24,27,29 Indeed, previous work has highlighted the independent modulation of cortical activity by locomotion and global arousal state as measured by pupil diameter,28 and recent findings have further detailed a tight association between periods of moderate arousal, facial movement, and cholinergic signaling in the cortex.24 Surprisingly, we found that PM neurons were more strongly modulated by changes in behavioral state, as measured by locomotion, facial motion, and pupil dilation, than the general population of V1 neurons or any individual direct afferent pathway. This strong representation of behavioral state information by PM neurons may facilitate the previously reported non-visual roles of PM in visually guided tasks.68,69 LP inputs to PM exhibited more robust sensitivity to behavioral-state variables than either V1 or LM inputs, suggesting that the higher-order thalamus may provide a key source of state information to PM. Indeed, LP thalamocortical activity was robustly linked to global arousal, as assessed by pupil diameter, and optogenetic inactivation of LP terminals reduced the modulation of postsynaptic PM neurons by pupil dilation but had no effect on modulation by locomotion. Consistent with a contextual role for LP, recent work found that LP thalamocortical axons projecting to the higher visual cortex do not encode a detailed representation of optic flow, which is signaled by corticocortical axons, but rather encode discrepancies between optic flow and self-motion.22 Corticocortical afferents may thus be a principal conduit of feedforward sensory information between cortical areas, whereas higher-order thalamocortical axons may primarily contribute state-dependent contextual modulation of this sensory signaling.70,71
Deep-layer cortical projection neurons may be a major source of the state-dependent modulation of LP activity. Although V1 projects to both PM and LP, these pathways arise from distinct populations of projection neurons with different properties.31,72 We found that V1 layer 5 corticothalamic neurons that project to LP are more strongly modulated by the behavioral state than corticocortical projection neurons. Previous work has likewise highlighted a potential role for corticothalamic projections in conveying locomotion modulation signals from the V1 to the LGN.34 Additional presynaptic sources may further amplify the unique state-dependent properties of LP thalamocortical neurons. In addition, some state-dependent modulation of LP activity may arise from the superior colliculus, which regulates pupil dilation and orienting responses.73,74 Previous work has identified several neuromodulatory systems75,76 and GABAergic pathways77,78 that selectively interact with higher-order thalamocortical neurons. Global arousal information may also reach LP via corticothalamic neurons in the deepest part of cortical layer 6, which project exclusively to the higher-order thalamus79 (see also Figures S4C and S4D). Deep layer 6 corticothalamic neurons are sensitive to the wake-promoting neuropeptide orexin,80 which is strongly coupled to pupil-linked arousal.81 Neuromodulatory signaling to corticothalamic neurons may thus regulate a selective channel for behavioral-state information from V1 to LP, potentially contributing to the strong correlation between pupil fluctuations and LP activity.
Our current results pertain to long-range synaptic inputs to a specific region of the higher-order mouse visual cortex, area PM. However, the mouse visual system consists of more than 10 distinct cortical processing regions downstream of V1.82 These regions have specific hierarchical relationships and communicate in both feedforward and feedback directions.9,37,83,84 Furthermore, analogously to the primate pulvinar,85 the mouse LP contains multiple retinotopically organized subdivisions.67 The functional role of LP in corticocortical communication may therefore differ depending on the hierarchical positions of the postsynaptic cortical regions.86 For instance, whereas LP input to V154 and medial higher visual areas (such as the PM) may primarily convey contextual and behavioral-state signals, LP input to lateral higher visual areas (such as the post-rhinal cortex) may instead convey robust and precise visual information.87 LP thalamocortical neurons might also have distinct functions based upon which specific pre- and postsynaptic cortical areas they serve to connect. Indeed, previous work found that whereas LP afferents to anterior lateral visual cortex (AL) were sensitive to changes in optic flow and suppressed by locomotion, LP afferents to PM were variably modulated by both signals.22 Finally, our data highlight potential contributions of LP afferents to local circuits in layer 2/3 of PM, but previous studies have suggested that long-range projections to different layers may carry distinct information,22,88,89 raising the possibility of different roles for LP projections to layer 1 or 5 in PM. However, the synaptic mechanisms by which higher-order thalamocortical inputs are integrated with direct corticocortical inputs within the cortex and how this integration may contribute to circuit function in different areas or cortical layers remain to be explored.
Previous work has shown that activity in ascending neuromodulatory pathways, such as those releasing acetylcholine and norepinephrine, may be linked to pupil-related global arousal.30 Neuromodulatory pathways may also carry motor-related signals, such as locomotion and facial motion.24,90 Our current results indicate that higher-order thalamocortical axons can convey state-modulated signals reminiscent of canonical neuromodulatory pathways. Future work will be required to disentangle the precise contributions of these different afferent pathways to both global and local state-dependent reconfigurations of cortical networks. Overall, our findings identify cortico-cortical and higher-order thalamocortical inputs that convey distinct information streams to the higher-order cortex. We find that corticocortical pathways convey robust sensory information to area PM, whereas higher-order thalamocortical inputs from LP primarily provide behavioral-state information. Higher-order thalamocortical inputs may thus serve as a modulatory route by which contextual information shapes the dynamic functional interactions between cortical regions and the integration of sensory information to guide behavioral output.
Limitations of the study
In the current study, we measured the activity of deep-layer neurons in V1 by recording fluorescence in dendritic branches. Previous work has shown that nearly all dendritic calcium activity is driven by back-propagating action potentials, making this a good readout of somatic activity.50 However, it is possible that our analysis includes some activity originating in the dendrites in response to synaptic inputs. We imaged neurons and afferent corticocortical and thalamocortical axons in layer 2/3 of PM. Thalamocortical projections also target apical dendrites in superficial layer 1, and it is possible that distal dendritic inputs may convey distinct information to postsynaptic neurons in PM. However, it is likely that LP inputs to both layer 1 and layer 2/3 dendrites exert modulatory effects on PM neurons. It is possible that projections to layer 1 are over-represented in our histological dataset due to tropism of the viral vectors for particular classes of projection neurons. Finally, because calcium imaging does not provide direct insight into synaptic interactions, our data highlight functional connectivity between long-range projections and postsynaptic targets rather than synaptic connectivity. However, because our eOPN3 manipulation regulated the release of synaptic vesicles from presynaptic terminals within PM,56 the resulting data specifically highlight local interactions between corticocortical afferents, higher-order thalamocortical afferents, and PM neurons. Light transmission to deeper cortical layers may be reduced compared to superficial layers, suggesting that our optogenetic effects could primarily arise from the selective suppression of LP afferents to PM in superficial layers. Robust suppression of inputs can change the activity of postsynaptic neurons due to a loss of overall excitation rather than reduction in a selective input. To avoid this issue, we endeavored to use levels of optogenetic manipulation that allowed our data to remain within the physiological range of normal circuit function.
eOPN3 is now a commonly used tool for systems neuroscience, and recent results suggest a wide range of effective light stimuli for long-term activation. Initial results (Mahn et al.56) and more recent efforts have found a half-life of efficacy around 5–6 min in vivo. In the current study, we illuminated terminals in PM for 1 s at moderately high power (30 mW/mm2) every 2 min. This is within the range of other recent studies using eOPN3 for in vivo suppression of synaptic release. Recent studies have used a range of illumination protocols, including 4 s every 60 s, 30 s every 2.33 min, and 15 s every 5 min.91–93 Notably, substantial suppression has been observed even following short pulse durations at low power.92 It is highly unlikely that any optogenetic suppression will be complete,94 and we therefore do not expect that our optogenetic manipulation equates to a total silencing of afferents. However, the existing evidence overwhelmingly indicates that eOPN3 is effective over a moderately long time course such as that used in the current study.
We examined the modulation of afferent populations and PM neurons by behavioral state using pupil diameter, facial motion, and locomotion as indicators of changes in the internal state of the animal. In the analysis presented in the current study, visual response data were not separated according to behavioral state. Although we assessed state-dependent modulation during periods when visual stimuli were also presented, visual stimuli were randomly interspersed with respect to transitions in behavioral state and thus unlikely to bias results. The role of cortico-cortical and thalamocortical inputs in regulating state-dependent visual response gain is a topic of considerable interest for future exploration.
RESOURCE AVAILABILITY
Lead contact
Further information and requests for resources and reagents should be directed to Jessica A. Cardin ([email protected]).
Materials availability
No new reagents were generated as a result of this study.
Data and code availability
Data reported in this paper and any additional information required are available from the lead contact upon request.
All original code is available at https://github.com/cardin-higley-lab/CC_TC_Neske_Project.
Any additional information required to reanalyze the data reported in this paper is available from the lead contact upon request.
STAR★METHODS
EXPERIMENTAL MODEL AND SUBJECT DETAILS
Animals
All animal handling and maintenance was performed according to the regulations of the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee of the Yale University School of Medicine. Adult male and female C57BL/6J mice (The Jackson Laboratory, strain #000664) aged 10–14 weeks were kept on a 12h light/dark cycle, provided with food and water ad libitum, and housed individually following head-post implants. In a subset of experiments, adult male and female Ai9 mice (The Jackson Laboratory, strain #007909) were used. All experiments were performed during the light cycle.
METHOD DETAILS
Surgical procedures
Surgeries were performed in adult mice in a stereotaxic frame, anesthetized with 1–2% isoflurane mixed with pure oxygen. Eyes were protected throughout the surgery with ophthalmic ointment (Puralube). After providing an intraperitoneal injection of Carprofen (5 mg/kg), a midline incision along the scalp was made to expose the skull. Small burr holes in the skull were made in stereotaxic coordinates to introduce viral vectors into multiple brain regions of interest– (mm lateral [L], posterior [P], and ventral [V] from bregma) LP thalamus: 1.6 L, 2.06 P, 2.7 V; PM visual cortex (2 sites): 1.8L, 2.7P, 0.4V & 1.5L, 2.9P, 0.4V; V1 visual cortex (3 sites): 2.5L, 3.5P, 0.4V & 3.0L, 3.5P,0.4V & 2.5L, 4.0P, 0.4V; LM visual cortex: 4.0L, 3.5P, 0.4V. Pulled glass capillaries were connected to a microinjection system (Stoelting Quintessential Stereotaxic Injector) to deliver viral vectors into the brain at a rate of 40–70 nL per minute, with a total volume of 100–200 nL per injection site. To express the calcium indicator GCaMP6s in neuronal cell bodies or long-range projection axons either AAV5-Syn-GCaMP6s or AAV1-Syn-GCaMP6s (1×1013 gc/mL; Addgene #100843) was injected into the relevant brain region. For experiments requiring expression of the soma-targeted GCaMP variant (riboGCaMP) in PM cell bodies, AAV9-Syn-GCaMP6m-RPL10a (1×1013 gc/mL; Addgene #158777) was injected into PM. For experiments requiring Cre-dependent optogenetic silencing of axon terminals within PM while imaging PM cell bodies, CAV2-Cre (1×1013 gc/mL; CRNS Plateforme de Vectorologie de Montpellier) was mixed in a 1:10 ratio with AAV5-syn-GCaMP6s and injected into PM and AAV5-Syn-SIO-eOPN3-mScarlet (2×1013 gc/mL; Addgene #125713) was injected into either LP, V1, or LM. All viruses were allowed to express for at least 3 weeks prior to experiments.
For headpost implantation, mice were anesthetized with isoflurane and the scalp was cleaned with Betadine solution. An incision was made at the midline and the scalp resected to each side to leave an open area of the skull. After cleaning the skull and scoring it lightly with a surgical blade, a custom titanium head post was secured with C&B-Metabond (Butler Schein) with the left PM or V1 centered. Two skull screws (McMaster-Carr) were placed at the right anterior and posterior poles (bilateral to the injection site). A 3×3-mm craniotomy was made over the left PM or V1. A glass window made of a 3×3-mm square inner coverslip adhered with an ultraviolet-curing adhesive (Norland Products) to a 5 mm-diameter round outer coverslip (both #1, Warner Instruments) was inserted into the craniotomy and secured to the skull with Cyanoacrylate glue (Loctite). A circular ring was attached to the titanium headpost with glue, and additional Metabond was applied to cover any exposed skull and to cover each skull screw. Analgesics were given immediately after surgery (5 mg/kg Carprofen and 0.05 mg/kg Buprenorphine) and on the two following days to aid recovery. Mice were given a course of antibiotics (Sulfatrim, Butler Schein) to prevent infection and were allowed to recover for 3–5 days following implant surgery before beginning wheel training.
Histology
For histological preparation of brain sections, mice were deeply anesthetized with isoflurane and transcardially perfused with PBS followed by 4% PFA in PBS. Perfused brains were extracted and postfixed overnight at 4°C. 40-μm coronal sections were then prepared from the fixed brains using a vibratome (Leica VT1000 S). Free-floating sections were then mounted to microscope slides, coated with mounting medium (ProLong Gold Antifade Mountant with DAPI, Invitrogen) and sealed with cover glass. Epifluorescence and confocal images of brain sections were taken with a Zeiss Axio Imager 2 and Zeiss LSM 900, respectively.
Calcium imaging
Calcium indicators in neuronal cell bodies and axon terminals within the cortex were imaged with a 2-photon Movable Objective Microscope (MOM) equipped with a galvo-resonant scanner (Sutter Instruments) through a 25x, 1.05 NA objective (Olympus). 2-photon excitation of the calcium indicators was achieved by scanning the output of a Ti-sapphire laser (MaiTai eHP DeepSee, SpectraPhysics) tuned to 920 nm. Emitted fluorescence was collected by a PMT (H10770PA-40, Hamamatsu) and digitized into image series with ScanImage software (Vidrio) at ~30 Hz at a resolution of 256×256. To prevent light contamination from the display monitor, the microscope was enclosed in blackout material that extended to the headpost.
All calcium imaging was performed through a cranial window in awake, head-fixed mice that could freely run on a cylindrical wheel. Wheel speed was monitored by a magnetic angle sensor (Digikey). Before calcium imaging data were collected, mice were habituated to head-fixation for several days to ensure they spontaneously engaged in sustained locomotion bouts with normal posture. During calcium imaging experiments, the mouse’s face was illuminated with an IR LED array and facial video was acquired with a miniature CMOS camera (Blackfly s-USB3, Flir) at a frame rate of 10 Hz. All data acquisition signals (laser scanner frame times, wheel signal, and video camera frame times) were digitized at 5 kHz using a Power1401 data acquisition system (CED).
Imaging of cell bodies and axon terminals in layer 2/3 was performed at 150–350 μm depth relative to the brain surface, while imaging of apical dendrites in layer 1 was performed at 50–100 μm depth. For each mouse, 1–4 fields of view were imaged. During each session, spontaneous activity was collected for 10 min before the series of visual stimuli were presented, and 10 min after (20 min total) as the mouse freely moved on the wheel in front of a mean-luminance gray screen.
Optogenetic stimulation
For imaging sessions in which projection axons expressing the inhibitory opsin eOPN3 were optically stimulated, a two-session experimental design was used to compare conditions with and without optical stimulation. Since eOPN3 has a relatively long and potentially variable time constant for inactivation (~5 min56), it is not well suited to traditional experimental designs using optogenetics in which control periods and periods with optical stimulation are interleaved throughout a session. Imaging data were therefore first collected during a session in which no optical stimuli were given (“Dark” session). After the end of this control session, the mouse was returned to its home cage for ~2 h. This waiting period ensured that residual effects of neuronal adaptation to visual stimulation did not carry over from one session to the next. After the waiting period, the mouse was placed back on the imaging set-up and the same field of view was imaged for a second session (“LED” session). During this session, light from a green LED (M530L3–530 nm, Thorlabs) was triggered with a 1-s TTL pulse, directed through the epifluorescence path of the Sutter MOM and reflected through the objective to produce widefield illumination onto the cranial window (~1 mm spot diameter, ~30 mW/mm2). This 1-s optical stimulus was given every 2 min throughout the session during intermittent pauses in scanning. Comparisons between neuronal properties were then made between the first and second imaging session to assess the effects of optogenetic manipulation.
Cre-dependent ablation
For Cre-mediated ablation of specific corticocortical pathways and higher-order thalamocortical pathways projecting to area PM, we injected ~200 nL CAV2-Cre into PM and ~200 nL AAV5-flex-taCasp3-TEVp (7×1012 vg/mL) (Yang et al., 2013) into either LP or V1. For histological verification of caspase-mediated cell death in these projection neurons, these injections were performed in Ai9 animals with within-brain control injections: one hemisphere injected with CAV2-Cre in PM and caspase in V1/LP and the other hemisphere injected with CAV2-Cre in PM and equal volume of sterile saline in V1/LP. After 3 weeks of expression, histological brain sections were analyzed for tdTomato-positive cells in V1/LP of each hemisphere. For in vivo calcium imaging experiments, CAV2-Cre was mixed in a 1:10 ratio with AAV5-syn-GCaMP6s and injected into PM and either AAV5-flex-taCasp3-TEVp (experimental animals) or saline (control animals) was injected into V1/LP. Calcium imaging was performed after 3 weeks of viral expression.
Visual stimulus presentation
Visual stimuli were presented on a 43 cm × 24 cm gamma-corrected LCD monitor (model B206HQL Aymph, Acer) positioned 45° from the mouse’s midline, with the center of the monitor located 18 cm from the mouse’s eye. The monitor subtended ~100° azimuth/67° elevation of the mouse’s visual hemifield. A photodiode (model #SM1PD1B, Thorlabs) was fixed to the lower right corner of the monitor and connected to the Power1401 to allow precise recordings of visual stimulus onset times. Visual stimuli were generated and synchronized with the 60 Hz refresh rate of the monitor using MATLAB-based Psychtoolbox software.95 Visual stimuli were 2 s in duration and consisted of either drifting sine-wave gratings of varying orientation, drift direction, size, spatial frequency, or temporal frequency or random dot kinematograms (RDKs). RDKs consisted of randomly positioned white circles (4° diameter) on an isoluminant gray background, such that 20% of the background was occupied by the circles. Each circle was then allowed to drift (80°/s) in a random direction based on the value of percent motion coherence chosen for a given 2-s visual stimulus. For all types of visual stimuli, the time between the end of one visual stimulus and the onset of the next (i.e., inter-stimulus interval) was 5 s.
Data analysis
Cell counts and fluorescence intensity quantification in histological sections
Fluorescently labeled cells were counted manually in each histological section. Sections were aligned to bregma based on the detection of a small volume (<50 nL) of red retrobeads (Lumafluor) injected into the brain at the anterior-posterior coordinate of bregma immediately before transcardial perfusions as a fiducial marker. Fluorescently labeled cells were assigned to specific brain regions based on alignment with coronal sections in the Paxinos-Franklin mouse brain atlas and previously reported coordinates.96,97 To calculate the total cell density in each brain region, the total number of cells counted in that region was divided by an estimate of its total volume, computed by numerical integration of the cross-sectional areas of the portions of each section corresponding to the brain region. Normalized cell count as a function of anterior-posterior distance from bregma was calculated by dividing the cell count associated with a given histological section by the highest cell count among all the sections from a given animal. To calculate the relative proportions of projections to PM for each mouse, we put the total number of tdTomato-positive cells counted across all areas in the denominator and the total number of tdTomato-positive cells counted in V1 and LM in the numerator and multiplied by 100 to yield the percentage that V1 and LM contribute to the feedforward projections to PM. Fluorescence intensity of axonal or dendritic labeling was measured in 10- or 5-μm bins across the laminar depth of the cortex relative to the pia for whole-cortex and layer 1 measurements, respectively. Mean normalized laminar fluorescence intensity for a given animal was calculated by dividing all fluorescence intensities by the highest fluorescence intensity for that animal and then averaging resulting normalized fluorescence vs. depth data from all sections in that animal.
Wheel position and change-points
Wheel position was determined from the output of the linear angle detector. The circular wheel position variable was first transformed to the [−π, π] interval. The phases were then circularly unwrapped to yield running distance as a linear variable, and locomotion speed was computed as a differential of distance (cm/s). A change-point detection algorithm detected locomotion onset/offset times based on changes in standard deviation of speed. Locomotion onset or offset times were estimated from periods when the moving standard deviations, as determined in a 0.5-s window, exceeded or fell below an empirical threshold of 0.1. Locomotion trials were required to have average speed exceeding 0.5 cm/s and last longer than 1 s. Quiescence trials were required to last longer than 2 s and have an average speed <0.5 cm/s.
Quantification of calcium signals
Analysis of imaging data was performed using ImageJ and custom routines in MATLAB (Mathworks). Motion artifacts and drifts in the Ca2+ signal were corrected with the moco plug-in in ImageJ,98 and regions of interest (ROIs) were selected as previously described.31,72,99–101 All pixels in each ROI were averaged as a measure of fluorescence, and the neuropil signal was subtracted. ΔF/F was calculated as (F-F0)/F0, where F0 was the lowest 10% of values from the neuropil-subtracted trace for each session.
For some analyses (Figure 4; Figure S6), ΔF/F signals were deconvolved to estimate the times of underlying Ca2+ events. The AR1 FOOPSI algorithm in the Oasis toolbox102 was used to find the optimal convolution kernel, baseline fluorescence, and noise distribution. Ca2+ events were then detected as times at which the deconvolved signal increased beyond 3 standard deviations. Time-varying Ca2+ event rates were then computed as
(Equation 1) |
where indicates the time at which a Ca2+ event occurred for events in a recording, and is a sliding Gaussian window,
(Equation 2) |
where .
Cellular ROI selection
Image series collected from 2-photon calcium imaging were corrected for motion artifacts using the moco plug-in on ImageJ.98 ROIs corresponding to cell bodies or axon terminals within the imaging field of view were chosen with a semi-automated procedure using mean- or max-projections of the imaging series.99–101,103 All pixels within an ROI were averaged as a measure of fluorescence. To correct for fluorescence originating from neuropil potentially contaminating the ROI signal, a group of pixels corresponding to a ~20 μm region around each ROI (excluding all selected ROIs) was averaged and subtracted from the ROI signal at each time point:
(Equation 3) |
From these neuropil-corrected raw fluorescence traces for each ROI, calcium activity was quantified as :
(Equation 4) |
where is a sliding 10th percentile of the raw fluorescence distribution, with a sliding window 60 s in width.
For ROIs corresponding to axon terminals, additional efforts were made to minimize inclusion of redundant ROIs that putatively belong to the same neuron in the presynaptic source region. Whereas redundancies in cell body ROIs are highly unlikely (i.e., 2 or more ROIs corresponding to the same neuronal cell body), ROIs for axon terminals could suffer from redundancy due to axonal branching. In order to minimize the number of redundant axon terminal ROIs in our dataset, we used a procedure based on activity correlations. For this procedure, we first generated a dataset derived from axonal calcium imaging sessions consisting of axon segments that were unambiguously part of the same stretch of axon (“sub-ROIs”). For all such sub-ROI pairs within an imaging field of view, we then calculated the partial correlation of their activity throughout the entire imaging session, subtracting the mean value of all pairwise correlation values from each raw pairwise correlation. The resulting distribution of partial correlation values corresponds to the case in which all axon terminal ROIs in a dataset are actually from the same neuron. We then compared this distribution of sub-ROI partial correlation values with that of the partial correlation values between axon terminal ROIs that were considered to belong to different neurons based upon structural images (i.e., terminals that were non-contiguous within the field of view). Based on an analysis of the distributions of correlations for ROIs and sub-ROIs (Figure S1), a partial correlation value of 0.3 was the most conservative threshold for separating these distributions in our datasets. Thus, in our larger dataset of axon terminal ROIs, if any 2 ROIs exhibited partial correlations ≥0.3, one of the ROIs was discarded as putatively redundant and not included in further analysis. This method was also applied to eliminate putatively redundant ROIs from experiments in which we imaged apical dendrites from pyramidal neurons arborizing in layer 1 (Figure S1; Figure S4).
Quantification of visual responses and visual feature selectivity
Prior to imaging sessions used for characterizing neuronal visual responses, the population receptive field for the ROIs in a field of view was mapped by partitioning the LCD monitor into a 3×3 grid and presenting binarized Gaussian noise stimuli104 within each patch in the grid. In subsequent imaging experiments using a given field of view, the monitor was positioned such that it was centered at the location within the visual field that elicited the largest visual responses from the neuron ROIs during these initial receptive field mapping sessions. With the exception of size tuning and motion coherence protocols (see below), visual stimuli were ~40° in diameter.
For ROIs corresponding to cell bodies or axon terminals, visually evoked response magnitude was quantified as the mean z-scored during a 2-s visual stimulus, with the mean and standard deviation of the signal during a 1-s baseline period prior to visual stimulus onset used for z-scoring. The overall visual response magnitude for a given ROI was the mean visually evoked z-scored averaged across all visual stimulus presentations during an imaging session. ROIs were considered significantly visually responsive if values during visual stimulation periods were significantly larger than values during 1-s periods before visual stimulus onset (p < 0.05, Student’s t-test). Calculation of selectivity for different visual features is described below. Visual trials were not separated by behavioral state.
Orientation/direction selectivity.
To calculate an ROI’s selectivity for the orientation and direction of drifting sine-wave gratings, 12 different drift directions were presented during imaging sessions (evenly spaced between 0° and 352°). The temporal frequency of the gratings was 1 Hz and the spatial frequency was either 0.05, 0.085, or 0.12 cycles/°. Each combination of drift direction, temporal frequency, and spatial frequency was randomly repeated 10x during an imaging session. For each ROI, orientation selectivity was quantified by 105:
(Equation 5) |
where is the mean visual response magnitude to drift direction , and direction selectivity was quantified by :
(Equation 6) |
Spatial frequency, temporal frequency, & speed tuning.
To calculate an ROI’s tuning for visual spatial frequency (SF), temporal frequency (TF), and speed, the spatial and temporal frequencies of sine-wave gratings drifting at 0° were varied approximately in octaves (SF: 0.02, 0.04, 0.08, 0.16, 0.32 cycles/°; TF: 0.5, 1, 2, 4, 8, 15, 24 Hz) and each SF-TF combination was randomly repeated 15x during an imaging session. Each ROI’s response to a specific SF or TF was computed as the mean visual response magnitude for each presentation of that SF or TF. Different SF-TF combinations corresponded to different speeds (in °/s), which was calculated as the ratio between TF and SF. Each ROI’s response to a specific speed was thus computed as the mean visual response magnitude for a unique ratio between TF and SF. To quantify the preferred SF, TF, and speed for a given ROI, visual responses as a function of either SF, TF, or speed were fit to log-Gaussian models106:
(Equation 7) |
where is either SF, TF, or speed and , and are parameters fit by nonlinear optimization. SF, TF, and speed tuning curves were considered well-fit if the model R2 was greater than 0.8. In this model, corresponds to preferred SF, TF, or speed.
Size tuning.
To calculate an ROI’s tuning for visual stimulus size, the diameter of drifting sine-wave gratings within the population receptive field was varied between 7° and 75° in increments of 10°. The temporal frequency of the gratings was 1 Hz, the spatial frequency was either 0.05, 0.085, or 0.12 cycles/°, and the drift direction was in either of the 4 cardinal directions. Each combination of size, temporal frequency, spatial frequency, and drift direction was randomly repeated 4x during an imaging session. The amount of surround suppression exhibited by an ROI was calculated from its size-tuning curve as a suppression index ():
(Equation 8) |
where is the magnitude of the response for the maximum size in stimulus set (75°) and is the magnitude of the maximum response across all stimulus sizes.
Motion coherence selectivity.
To characterize an ROI’s selectivity for coherent motion within the visual field, full-screen random dot kinematograms (RDKs) (see “Visual stimulus presentation”) were presented in which the percent motion coherence was varied among 8 values (20, 25, 31, 40, 50, 63, 79, 100%). For each percent motion coherence value, drift direction could be in either of the 4 cardinal directions. Each combination of percent motion coherence and drift direction was randomly repeated 15x during an imaging session. For each ROI, the drift direction that elicited the largest visual responses was chosen as that ROI’s preferred direction and visual responses as a function of motion coherence were analyzed for that drift direction. To quantify an ROI’s selectivity for coherent visual motion, the Pearson correlation coefficient between a vector of visual responses for each motion coherence value and a vector of the corresponding motion coherence values was calculated.40
Quantification of modulation of neuronal activity by locomotion
For calculation of locomotion modulation index (LMI), only locomotion bouts occurring after and before at least 15 s of quiescence were used. LMI for each ROI was calculated as
(Equation 9) |
where is the mean during the 4 s following locomotion onset and is the mean during the 10–15 s after locomotion offset.
Quantification of modulation of neuronal activity by pupil size
From video of the mouse’s face, pupil size of the non-visually-stimulated eye was computed from individual video frames. Frames were first cropped to a region around the eye and image pixels were binarized such that the majority of the pixels within the pupil were white and the majority of the pixels outside of the pupil were black (the IR light from the Ti-sapphire laser that escapes through the mouses eye causes the pupil to appear white when captured by the camera). Canny edge detection was then used on the binarized image frames to detect edges of the pupil. Pupil size was then estimated as the authalic diameter of the area enclosed by the detected edges (i.e., the diameter of a circle with the same area).44
All analyses relating neuronal activity to pupil fluctuations were restricted to quiescent (i.e., non-locomotion) periods of imaging sessions. Visual stimuli were randomly presented throughout the session and these periods were not excluded from the overall analysis. To relate pupil dilation and constriction to fluctuations in neuronal activity, the time series of pupil size was bandpass-filtered between 0.1 and 1 Hz. The bandpass-filtered signal of pupil size was converted to phase of the pupil dilation-constriction cycle through a Hilbert transform, which transforms the signal into a time series of complex numbers. The inverse tangent of the ratio between the imaginary and real parts of the complex number at each time point yields the phase of the bandpass-filtered pupil signal. To quantify neuronal activity as a function of the phase of the dilation-constriction cycle, time series for each ROI were also band-pass-filtered between 0.1 and 1 Hz. These bandpass-filtered signals were then z-scored using baseline periods associated with minimal changes in pupil size and small baseline pupil diameter. Specifically, these baseline periods needed to be associated with a mean of the bandpass-filtered pupil signal that was less than 1.5x the interquartile range of this signal and a mean pupil size that was within the bottom 25th percentile of the distribution of pupil sizes observed during quiescent periods during a given session. The mean and standard deviation of the bandpass-filtered signal during these baseline periods was used for z-scoring the signal. For each dilation-constriction cycle, z-scored for each ROI was then averaged in bins of width π/32 from −π to π. Only dilation-constriction periods larger than a threshold of 1.5x the interquartile range of the bandpass-filtered pupil signal were analyzed.
To calculate cross-correlations between neuronal activity and pupil fluctuations, both the time series of pupil size and the time series of for each ROI were lowpass-filtered at 10 Hz. Correlations between these signals were then computed at different lags in 100-ms increments in a time window of 8 s, using the pupil signal as the reference signal. The strength of the modulation of by pupil size for each ROI was quantified as the value of the peak cross-correlation between and pupil size.
Quantification of modulation of neuronal activity by facial motion/whisking
From video of the mouse’s face, a region consisting of the nose, mouth, and whisker pads was cropped from each frame. A region of interest was then drawn around the whisker pad and facial motion energy was calculated as the absolute value of the difference in average pixel intensity of the whisker pad between successive image frames. Onset times of facial motion bouts were computed by first z-scoring the entire time series of facial motion energy. High and low thresholds of facial motion energy were then defined as the 60% and 40% quantiles of the z-scored facial motion energy distribution during quiescent periods. After smoothing the time series of z-scored facial motion energy with a 1-s moving-average filter, high and low facial movement states were defined as periods in which the smoothed signal remained above or below the high and low threshold levels for at least 500 ms. Onset of a facial motion bout was then defined as the time at which a high facial motion state was preceded by at least 4 s of a low facial motion state. ROI calcium activity aligned to these onsets was z-scored using the mean and standard deviation of the during low facial motion periods. As for pupil diameter, facial motion data were measured during imaging sessions where visual stimuli were randomly presented.
To calculate cross-correlations between neuronal activity and facial motion, both the time series of facial motion energy and the time series of for each ROI were lowpass-filtered at 10 Hz. Correlations between these signals were then computed at different lags in 100-ms increments in a time window of 8 s, using the facial motion energy as the reference signal. The strength of the modulation of by facial motion for each ROI was quantified as the value of the peak cross-correlation between and facial motion energy.
Calculation of Ca2+ event-triggered averages
For data shown in Figures 4E and 4F and Figures S6B and S6C, Ca2+ activity aligned to cell body Ca2+ events was computed as follows. For each riboGCaMP6m-expressing cell body identified in a field of view, the times of its Ca2+ events were estimated from the deconvolved ΔF/F signal. In an 8-s window around each Ca2+ event from the cell body, the population average Ca2+ event rate from all other relevant ROIs (i.e., axon ROIs or ROIs from other cell bodies) was calculated. For each cell body whose event-triggered average was calculated this way, the peak event rate was taken as the maximum event rate in the 8-s window.
Statistical tests
In both the text and figures “N” refers to number of mice and “n” refers to number of neuron ROIs (cell bodies or axon terminals). Unless otherwise noted, data are summarized by either box-and-whisker charts (with “boxes” marking the median value and the 25% and 75% quantiles, and “whiskers” marking the minimum and maximum values in the dataset) or summary curves indicating the mean value and shading indicating the standard error of the mean.
For statistical comparisons among groups, we accounted for the nested structure of the data (i.e., cellular ROIs nested within mice). Without taking nesting into consideration, false positive rates are inflated since cellular ROIs from the same mouse cannot be considered independent measurements.107 We used semi-weighted error estimators to account for the nesting of cellular ROIs within mice.72 For this procedure, let be the mean for a given parameter (e.g., locomotion modulation index) for the -th mouse in a group, where is the parameter value for the -th ROI in a set if ROIs for mouse :
(Equation 10) |
The unweighted estimator of the mean is defined as the mean of for all mice in a group of mice, where is the number of degrees of freedom. This analysis is suboptimal, as some animals have many cells and yield more reliable estimates, whereas other animals have few cells and yield less reliable estimates. At the other extreme, the weighted estimator is defined by pooling parameter values from all ROIs across the mice in a group and using this number as the degrees of freedom. While the weighted estimator is commonly used in statistical comparisons, the false positive rate is improperly controlled in this case. The semi-weighted estimator for a given parameter measured from multiple ROIs from multiple mice in a group is defined as:
(Equation 11) |
where the weight for the -th mouse is defined as
(Equation 12) |
and is the within-mouse variance across ROIs and is the estimated variance across all mice within the group using maximum likelihood estimation108:
(Equation 13) |
Equations 12 and 13 were solved iteratively and checked for convergence of to <0.001. If there is minimal across-mouse variance, then the contribution of each ROI is weighted by the number of mice (the weighted estimator), whereas if across-mouse variance is large (i.e., parameter measurements for ROIs within a mouse are strongly dependent), then each mouse is given the same weight.
The semi-weighted estimators derived for different cell types (e.g., V1 axons vs. LP axons) or experimental conditions (e.g., control vs. eOPN3) were then used to calculate the statistical significance of pairwise comparisons using a Student’s t-test. The Benjamini-Hochberg procedure was used to control the false discovery rate from multiple pairwise comparisons. p-values <0.05 after multiple comparisons correction were considered statistically significant. Central markers on box-whisker plots indicate median values, boxes delineate the 25th and 75th percentiles, and upper/lower whiskers indicate maximum/minimum values.
QUANTIFICATION AND STATISTICAL ANALYSIS
Data were analyzed using custom-written scripts in Mathematica 11 (Wolfram Research) and MATLAB R2018a (Mathworks). Statistical tests, exact values of n, and statistical values for all analyses are listed in Table S1.
Supplementary Material
KEY RESOURCES TABLE.
REAGENT or RESOURCE | SOURCE | IDENTIFIER |
---|---|---|
| ||
Bacterial and virus strains | ||
| ||
AAV5-Syn-GCaMP6s | Chenetal., 201367 | Addgene #100843 |
AAV1-Syn-GCaMP6s | Chenetal., 201367 | Addgene #100843 |
CAV2-Cre | CRNS Plateforme de Vectorologie de Montpellier | https://plateau-igmm.pvm.cnrs.fr/ |
AAV5 ef1a-Flex-taCasP3-TEVP | Yang et al., 201347 | UNC Vector Core |
AAV5-Syn-SIO-eOPN3-mScarlet | Mahn et al., 202156 | Addgene #125713 |
| ||
Experimental models: Organisms/strains | ||
| ||
Mouse Ai9 | Jackson Laboratory | JAX 007909 |
Mouse c57Bl/6 | Jackson Laboratory | JAX 000664 |
| ||
Software and algorithms | ||
| ||
MATLAB R2022b (with curve fitting toolbox) | Mathworks | https://www.mathworks.com/ |
Mathematica 11 | Wolfram Research | https://www.wolfram.com/mathematica/ |
ImageJ Moco Algorithm | Dubbs et al., 201668 | https://github.com/NTCColumbia/moco |
Custom code | This paper |
https://github.com/cardin-higley-lab/CC_TC_Neske_Project
https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.13899503 |
Highlights.
Projections from LP to higher-order cortex PM exhibit strong state modulation
LP projections contribute to PM neuron state modulation but not visual responses
Corticocortical projections to PM convey visual, but not state, information
The impact of LP on PM activity is consistent with a modulatory role
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
The authors thank all members of the Higley and Cardin laboratories for helpful input throughout all stages of this study. We thank Rima Pant for generating the AAV vectors. We thank Dr. Ofer Yizhar for the gift of the eOPN3 plasmid. This work was supported by funding from the NIH (R01EY022951 and R01MH113852 to J.A.C., F32NS100279 and K99EY030550 to G.T.N., and EY026878 to the Yale Vision Core), an award from the Kavli Institute of Neuroscience (to J.A.C.), and support from the Ludwig Foundation (to J.A.C.).
Footnotes
DECLARATION OF INTERESTS
The authors declare no competing interests.
SUPPLEMENTAL INFORMATION
Supplemental information can be found online at https://doi.org/10.1016/j.celrep.2025.115292.
REFERENCES
- 1.Riesenhuber M, and Poggio T (1999). Hierarchical models of object recognition in cortex. Nat. Neurosci. 2, 1019–1025. 10.1038/14819. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Orban GA (2008). Higher order visual processing in macaque extrastriate cortex. Physiol. Rev. 88, 59–89. 10.1152/physrev.00008.2007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Mishkin M, and Ungerleider LG (1982). Contribution of striate inputs to the visuospatial functions of parieto-preoccipital cortex in monkeys. Behav. Brain Res. 6, 57–77. 10.1016/0166-4328(82)90081-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Felleman DJ, and Van Essen DC (1991). Distributed hierarchical processing in the primate cerebral cortex. Cerebr. Cortex 1, 1–47. 10.1093/cercor/1.1.1-a. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Gamanut R, Kennedy H, Toroczkai Z, Ercsey-Ravasz M, Van Essen DC, Knoblauch K, and Burkhalter A (2018). The Mouse Cortical Connectome, Characterized by an Ultra-Dense Cortical Graph, Maintains Specificity by Distinct Connectivity Profiles. Neuron 97, 698–715. 10.1016/j.neuron.2017.12.037. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Zingg B, Hintiryan H, Gou L, Song MY, Bay M, Bienkowski MS, Foster NN, Yamashita S, Bowman I, Toga AW, and Dong HW (2014). Neural networks of the mouse neocortex. Cell 156, 1096–1111. 10.1016/j.cell.2014.02.023. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Petreanu L, Mao T, Sternson SM, and Svoboda K (2009). The subcellular organization of neocortical excitatory connections. Nature 457, 1142–1145. 10.1038/nature07709. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Harris KD, and Shepherd GMG (2015). The neocortical circuit: themes and variations. Nat. Neurosci. 18, 170–181. 10.1038/nn.3917. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.D’Souza RD, Meier AM, Bista P, Wang Q, and Burkhalter A (2016). Recruitment of inhibition and excitation across mouse visual cortex depends on the hierarchy of interconnecting areas. Elife 5, e19332. 10.7554/eLife.19332. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Haider B, and McCormick DA (2009). Rapid neocortical dynamics: cellular and network mechanisms. Neuron 62, 171–189. 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.04.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Kohn A, Jasper AI, Semedo JD, Gokcen E, Machens CK, and Yu BM (2020). Principles of Corticocortical Communication: Proposed Schemes and Design Considerations. Trends Neurosci. 43, 725–737. 10.1016/j.tins.2020.07.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.El-Shamayleh Y, Kumbhani RD, Dhruv NT, and Movshon JA (2013). Visual response properties of V1 neurons projecting to V2 in macaque. J. Neurosci. 33, 16594–16605. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2753-13.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Glickfeld LL, Andermann ML, Bonin V, and Reid RC (2013). Cortico-cortical projections in mouse visual cortex are functionally target specific. Nat. Neurosci. 16, 219–226. 10.1038/nn.3300. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Movshon JA, and Newsome WT (1996). Visual response properties of striate cortical neurons projecting to area MT in macaque monkeys. J. Neurosci. 16, 7733–7741. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.16-23-07733.1996. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Shipp S (2003). The functional logic of cortico-pulvinar connections. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 358, 1605–1624. 10.1098/rstb.2002.1213. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Sherman SM, and Guillery RW (2011). Distinct functions for direct and transthalamic corticocortical connections. J. Neurophysiol. 106, 1068–1077. 10.1152/jn.00429.2011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Groh A, de Kock CPJ, Wimmer VC, Sakmann B, and Kuner T (2008). Driver or coincidence detector: modal switch of a corticothalamic giant synapse controlled by spontaneous activity and short-term depression. J. Neurosci. 28, 9652–9663. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1554-08.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Li J, Guido W, and Bickford ME (2003). Two distinct types of corticothalamic EPSPs and their contribution to short-term synaptic plasticity. J. Neurophysiol. 90, 3429–3440. 10.1152/jn.00456.2003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Reichova I, and Sherman SM (2004). Somatosensory corticothalamic projections: distinguishing drivers from modulators. J. Neurophysiol. 92, 2185–2197. 10.1152/jn.00322.2004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Tohmi M, Meguro R, Tsukano H, Hishida R, and Shibuki K (2014). The extrageniculate visual pathway generates distinct response properties in the higher visual areas of mice. Curr. Biol. 24, 587–597. 10.1016/j.cub.2014.01.061. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Theyel BB, Llano DA, and Sherman SM (2010). The corticothalamocortical circuit drives higher-order cortex in the mouse. Nat. Neurosci. 13, 84–88. 10.1038/nn.2449. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Blot A, Roth MM, Gasler I, Javadzadeh M, Imhof F, and Hofer SB (2021). Visual intracortical and transthalamic pathways carry distinct information to cortical areas. Neuron 109, 1996–2008.e6. 10.1016/j.neuron.2021.04.017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Benisty H, Barson D, Moberly AH, Lohani S, Tang L, Coifman RR, Crair MC, Mishne G, Cardin JA, and Higley MJ (2024). Rapid fluctuations in functional connectivity of cortical networks encode spontaneous behavior. Nat. Neurosci. 27, 148–158. 10.1038/s41593-023-01498-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Lohani S, Moberly AH, Benisty H, Landa B, Jing M, Li Y, Higley MJ, and Cardin JA (2022). Spatiotemporally heterogeneous coordination of cholinergic and neocortical activity. Nat. Neurosci. 25, 1706–1713. 10.1038/s41593-022-01202-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.McGinley MJ, Vinck M, Reimer J, Batista-Brito R, Zagha E, Cadwell CR, Tolias AS, Cardin JA, and McCormick DA (2015). Waking State: Rapid Variations Modulate Neural and Behavioral Responses. Neuron 87, 1143–1161. 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.09.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Salkoff DB, Zagha E, McCarthy E, and McCormick DA (2020). Movement and Performance Explain Widespread Cortical Activity in a Visual Detection Task. Cerebr. Cortex 30, 421–437. 10.1093/cercor/bhz206. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Stringer C, Pachitariu M, Steinmetz N, Reddy CB, Carandini M, and Harris KD (2019). Spontaneous behaviors drive multidimensional, brainwide activity. Science 364, 255. 10.1126/science.aav7893. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Vinck M, Batista-Brito R, Knoblich U, and Cardin JA (2015). Arousal and locomotion make distinct contributions to cortical activity patterns and visual encoding. Neuron 86, 740–754. 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.03.028. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Musall S, Kaufman MT, Juavinett AL, Gluf S, and Churchland AK (2019). Single-trial neural dynamics are dominated by richly varied movements. Nat. Neurosci. 22, 1677–1686. 10.1038/s41593-019-0502-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Reimer J, Froudarakis E, Cadwell CR, Yatsenko D, Denfield GH, and Tolias AS (2014). Pupil Fluctuations Track Fast Switching of Cortical States during Quiet Wakefulness. Neuron 84, 355–362. 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.09.033. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Tang L, and Higley MJ (2020). Layer 5 Circuits in V1 Differentially Control Visuomotor Behavior. Neuron 105, 346–354.e5. 10.1016/j.neuron.2019.10.014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Niell CM, and Stryker MP (2010). Modulation of Visual Responses by Behavioral State in Mouse Visual Cortex. Neuron 65, 472–479. 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.01.033. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Spacek MA, Crombie D, Bauer Y, Born G, Liu X, Katzner S, and Busse L (2022). Robust effects of corticothalamic feedback and behavioral state on movie responses in mouse dLGN. Elife 11, e70469. 10.7554/eLife.70469. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Reinhold K, Resulaj A, and Scanziani M (2023). Brain State-Dependent Modulation of Thalamic Visual Processing by Cortico-Thalamic Feedback. J. Neurosci. 43, 1540–1554. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2124-21.2022. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Molnar B, Sere P, Borde S, Koos K, Zsigri N, Horvath P, and Lorincz ML (2021). Cell Type-Specific Arousal-Dependent Modulation of Thalamic Activity in the Lateral Geniculate Nucleus. Cereb Cortex Commun 2, tgab020. 10.1093/texcom/tgab020. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Siegle JH, Jia X, Durand S, Gale S, Bennett C, Graddis N, Heller G, Ramirez TK, Choi H, Luviano JA, et al. (2021). Survey of spiking in the mouse visual system reveals functional hierarchy. Nature 592, 86–92. 10.1038/s41586-020-03171-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Wang Q, Gao E, and Burkhalter A (2011). Gateways of ventral and dorsal streams in mouse visual cortex. J. Neurosci. 31, 1905–1918. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3488-10.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Wang Q, Sporns O, and Burkhalter A (2012). Network analysis of corticocortical connections reveals ventral and dorsal processing streams in mouse visual cortex. J. Neurosci. 32, 4386–4399. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.6063-11.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Junyent F, and Kremer EJ (2015). CAV-2–why a canine virus is a neurobiologist’s best friend. Curr. Opin. Pharmacol. 24, 86–93. 10.1016/j.coph.2015.08.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Sit KK, and Goard MJ (2020). Distributed and retinotopically asymmetric processing of coherent motion in mouse visual cortex. Nat. Commun. 11, 3565. 10.1038/s41467-020-17283-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Murgas KA, Wilson AM, Michael V, and Glickfeld LL (2020). Unique Spatial Integration in Mouse Primary Visual Cortex and Higher Visual Areas. J. Neurosci. 40, 1862–1873. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1997-19.2020. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Fu Y, Tucciarone JM, Espinosa JS, Sheng N, Darcy DP, Nicoll RA, Huang ZJ, and Stryker MP (2014). A cortical circuit for gain control by behavioral state. Cell 156, 1139–1152. 10.1016/j.cell.2014.01.050. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Polack PO, Friedman J, and Golshani P (2013). Cellular mechanisms of brain state-dependent gain modulation in visual cortex. Nat. Neurosci. 16, 1331–1339. 10.1038/nn.3464. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Neske GT, Nestvogel D, Steffan PJ, and McCormick DA (2019). Distinct Waking States for Strong Evoked Responses in Primary Visual Cortex and Optimal Visual Detection Performance. J. Neurosci. 39, 10044–10059. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1226-18.2019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Nestvogel DB, and McCormick DA (2022). Visual thalamocortical mechanisms of waking state-dependent activity and alpha oscillations. Neuron 110, 120–138.e4. 10.1016/j.neuron.2021.10.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Poulet JFA, Fernandez LMJ, Crochet S, and Petersen CCH (2012). Thalamic control of cortical states. Nat. Neurosci. 15, 370–372. 10.1038/nn.3035. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Zagha E, Casale AE, Sachdev RNS, McGinley MJ, and McCormick DA (2013). Motor cortex feedback influences sensory processing by modulating network state. Neuron 79, 567–578. 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.06.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Varela C (2014). Thalamic neuromodulation and its implications for executive networks. Front. Neural Circ. 8, 69. 10.3389/fncir.2014.00069. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.McCormick DA (1992). Neurotransmitter actions in the thalamus and cerebral cortex and their role in neuromodulation of thalamocortical activity. Prog. Neurobiol. 39, 337–388. 10.1016/0301-0082(92)90012-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Beaulieu-Laroche L, Toloza EHS, Brown NJ, and Harnett MT (2019). Widespread and Highly Correlated Somato-dendritic Activity in Cortical Layer 5 Neurons. Neuron 103, 235–241.e4. 10.1016/j.neuron.2019.05.014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Peters AJ, Lee J, Hedrick NG, O’Neil K, and Komiyama T (2017). Reorganization of corticospinal output during motor learning. Nat. Neurosci. 20, 1133–1141. 10.1038/nn.4596. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Chen Y, Jang H, Spratt PWE, Kosar S, Taylor DE, Essner RA, Bai L, Leib DE, Kuo TW, Lin YC, et al. (2020). Soma-Targeted Imaging of Neural Circuits by Ribosome Tethering. Neuron 107, 454–469.e6. 10.1016/j.neuron.2020.05.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Miller-Hansen AJ, and Sherman SM (2022). Conserved patterns of functional organization between cortex and thalamus in mice. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 119, e2201481119. 10.1073/pnas.2201481119. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Roth MM, Dahmen JC, Muir DR, Imhof F, Martini FJ, and Hofer SB (2016). Thalamic nuclei convey diverse contextual information to layer 1 of visual cortex. Nat. Neurosci. 19, 299–307. 10.1038/nn.4197. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Li JY, Hass CA, Matthews I, Kristl AC, and Glickfeld LL (2021). Distinct recruitment of feedforward and recurrent pathways across higher-order areas of mouse visual cortex. Curr. Biol. 31, 5024–5036.e5. 10.1016/j.cub.2021.09.042. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Mahn M, Saraf-Sinik I, Patil P, Pulin M, Bitton E, Karalis N, Bruentgens F, Palgi S, Gat A, Dine J, et al. (2021). Efficient optogenetic silencing of neurotransmitter release with a mosquito rhodopsin. Neuron 109, 1621–1635.e8. 10.1016/j.neuron.2021.03.013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Crandall SR, Cruikshank SJ, and Connors BW (2015). A corticothalamic switch: controlling the thalamus with dynamic synapses. Neuron 86, 768–782. 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.03.040. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Merabet L, Desautels A, Minville K, and Casanova C (1998). Motion integration in a thalamic visual nucleus. Nature 396, 265–268. 10.1038/24382. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Schmitt LI, Wimmer RD, Nakajima M, Happ M, Mofakham S, and Halassa MM (2017). Thalamic amplification of cortical connectivity sustains attentional control. Nature 545, 219–223. 10.1038/nature22073. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Van Essen DC (2005). Corticocortical and thalamocortical information flow in the primate visual system. Prog. Brain Res. 149, 173–185. 10.1016/S0079-6123(05)49013-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Fiebelkorn IC, Pinsk MA, and Kastner S (2019). The mediodorsal pulvinar coordinates the macaque fronto-parietal network during rhythmic spatial attention. Nat. Commun. 10, 215. 10.1038/s41467-018-08151-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Saalmann YB, Pinsk MA, Wang L, Li X, and Kastner S (2012). The pulvinar regulates information transmission between cortical areas based on attention demands. Science 337, 753–756. 10.1126/science.1223082. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Zhou H, Schafer RJ, and Desimone R (2016). Pulvinar-Cortex Interactions in Vision and Attention. Neuron 89, 209–220. 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.11.034. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Andermann ML, Kerlin AM, Roumis DK, Glickfeld LL, and Reid RC (2011). Functional specialization of mouse higher visual cortical areas. Neuron 72, 1025–1039. 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.11.013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Roth MM, Helmchen F, and Kampa BM (2012). Distinct functional properties of primary and posteromedial visual area of mouse neocortex. J. Neurosci. 32, 9716–9726. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0110-12.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Marshel JH, Garrett ME, Nauhaus I, and Callaway EM (2011). Functional specialization of seven mouse visual cortical areas. Neuron 72, 1040–1054. 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.12.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Bennett C, Arroyo S, and Hestrin S (2013). Subthreshold Mechanisms Underlying State-Dependent Modulation of Visual Responses. Neuron 80, 350–357. 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.08.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Jin M, and Glickfeld LL (2020). Mouse Higher Visual Areas Provide Both Distributed and Specialized Contributions to Visually Guided Behaviors. Curr. Biol. 30, 4682–4692.e7. 10.1016/j.cub.2020.09.015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Goltstein PM, Reinert S, Bonhoeffer T, and Hübener M (2021). Mouse visual cortex areas represent perceptual and semantic features of learned visual categories. Nat. Neurosci. 24, 1441–1451. 10.1038/s41593-021-00914-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.La Terra D, Bjerre AS, Rosier M, Masuda R, Ryan TJ, and Palmer LM (2022). The role of higher-order thalamus during learning and correct performance in goal-directed behavior. Elife 11, e77177. 10.7554/eLife.77177. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Petty GH, Kinnischtzke AK, Hong YK, and Bruno RM (2021). Effects of arousal and movement on secondary somatosensory and visual thalamus. Elife 10, e67611. 10.7554/eLife.67611. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Lur G, Vinck MA, Tang L, Cardin JA, and Higley MJ (2016). Projection-Specific Visual Feature Encoding by Layer 5 Cortical Subnetworks . Cell Rep. 14, 2538–2545. 10.1016/j.celrep.2016.02.050. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Joshi S, Li Y, Kalwani RM, and Gold JI (2016). Relationships between Pupil Diameter and Neuronal Activity in the Locus Coeruleus, Colliculi, and Cingulate Cortex. Neuron 89, 221–234. 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.11.028. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Wang CA, Boehnke SE, White BJ, and Munoz DP (2012). Microstimulation of the monkey superior colliculus induces pupil dilation without evoking saccades. J. Neurosci. 32, 3629–3636. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5512-11.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75.Varela C, and Sherman SM (2007). Differences in response to muscarinic activation between first and higher order thalamic relays. J. Neurophysiol. 98, 3538–3547. 10.1152/jn.00578.2007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76.Varela C, and Sherman SM (2009). Differences in response to serotonergic activation between first and higher order thalamic nuclei. Cerebr. Cortex 19, 1776–1786. 10.1093/cercor/bhn208. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77.Bokor H, Frère SGA, Eyre MD, Slézia A, Ulbert I, Lüthi A, and Acsády L (2005). Selective GABAergic control of higher-order thalamic relays. Neuron 45, 929–940. 10.1016/j.neuron.2005.01.048. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 78.Martinez-Garcia RI, Voelcker B, Zaltsman JB, Patrick SL, Stevens TR, Connors BW, and Cruikshank SJ (2020). Two dynamically distinct circuits drive inhibition in the sensory thalamus. Nature 583, 813–818. 10.1038/s41586-020-2512-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79.Hoerder-Suabedissen A, Hayashi S, Upton L, Nolan Z, Casas-Torremocha D, Grant E, Viswanathan S, Kanold PO, Clasca F, Kim Y, and Molná r Z. (2018). Subset of Cortical Layer 6b Neurons Selectively Innervates Higher Order Thalamic Nuclei in Mice. Cerebr. Cortex 28, 1882–1897. 10.1093/cercor/bhy036. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 80.Bayer L, Serafin M, Eggermann E, Saint-Mleux B, Machard D, Jones BE, and Mühlethaler M (2004). Exclusive postsynaptic action of hypocretin-orexin on sublayer 6b cortical neurons. J. Neurosci. 24, 6760–6764. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1783-04.2004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 81.Grujic N, Tesmer A, Bracey E, Peleg-Raibstein D, and Burdakov D (2023). Control and coding of pupil size by hypothalamic orexin neurons. Nat. Neurosci. 26, 1160–1164. 10.1038/s41593-023-01365-w. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 82.Zhuang J, Ng L, Williams D, Valley M, Li Y, Garrett M, and Waters J (2017). An extended retinotopic map of mouse cortex. Elife 6, e18372. 10.7554/eLife.18372. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 83.Glickfeld LL, and Olsen SR (2017). Higher-Order Areas of the Mouse Visual Cortex. Annu. Rev. Vis. Sci. 3, 251–273. 10.1146/annurev-vision-102016-061331. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 84.Wang Q, and Burkhalter A (2007). Area map of mouse visual cortex. J. Comp. Neurol. 502, 339–357. 10.1002/cne.21286. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 85.Kaas JH, and Lyon DC (2007). Pulvinar contributions to the dorsal and ventral streams of visual processing in primates. Brain Res. Rev. 55, 285–296. 10.1016/j.brainresrev.2007.02.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 86.de Souza BOF, Cortes N, and Casanova C (2020). Pulvinar Modulates Contrast Responses in the Visual Cortex as a Function of Cortical Hierarchy. Cerebr. Cortex 30, 1068–1086. 10.1093/cercor/bhz149. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 87.Beltramo R, and Scanziani M (2019). A collicular visual cortex: Neocortical space for an ancient midbrain visual structure. Science 363, 64–69. 10.1126/science.aau7052. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 88.Purushothaman G, Marion R, Li K, and Casagrande VA (2012). Gating and control of primary visual cortex by pulvinar. Nat. Neurosci. 15, 905–912. 10.1038/nn.3106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 89.Liu Y, Zhang J, Jiang Z, Qin M, Xu M, Zhang S, and Ma G (2024). Organization of corticocortical and thalamocortical top-down inputs in the primary visual cortex. Nat. Commun. 15, 4495. 10.1038/s41467-024-48924-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 90.Collins L, Francis J, Emanuel B, and McCormick DA (2023). Cholinergic and noradrenergic axonal activity contains a behavioral-state signal that is coordinated across the dorsal cortex. Elife 12, e81826. 10.7554/eLife.81826. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 91.Zangen E, Hadar S, Lawrence C, Obeid M, Rasras H, Hanzin E, Aslan O, Zur E, Schulcz N, Cohen-Hatab D, et al. (2024). Prefrontal cortex neurons encode ambient light intensity differentially across regions and layers. Nat. Commun. 15, 5501. 10.1038/s41467-024-49794-w. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 92.Ren C, Peng K, Yang R, Liu W, Liu C, and Komiyama T (2022). Global and subtype-specific modulation of cortical inhibitory neurons regulated by acetylcholine during motor learning. Neuron 110, 2334–2350.e8. 10.1016/j.neuron.2022.04.031. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 93.Dubanet O, and Higley MJ (2024). Retrosplenial inputs drive visual representations in the medial entorhinal cortex. Cell Rep. 43, 114470. 10.1016/j.celrep.2024.114470. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 94.Cardin JA (2012). Dissecting local circuits in vivo: integrated optogenetic and electrophysiology approaches for exploring inhibitory regulation of cortical activity. J. Physiol. Paris 106, 104–111. 10.1016/j.jphysparis.2011.09.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 95.Brainard DH (1997). The Psychophysics Toolbox. Spatial Vis. 10, 433–436. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 96.Franklin KBJ, and Paxinos G (2013). Paxinos and Franklin’s the Mouse Brain in Stereotaxic Coordinates, Fourth edition (Academic Press, an imprint of Elsevier). [Google Scholar]
- 97.Minderer M, Brown KD, and Harvey CD (2019). The Spatial Structure of Neural Encoding in Mouse Posterior Cortex during Navigation. Neuron 102, 232–248.e11. 10.1016/j.neuron.2019.01.029. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 98.Dubbs A, Guevara J, and Yuste R (2016). moco: Fast Motion Correction for Calcium Imaging. Front. Neuroinf. 10, 6. 10.3389/fninf.2016.00006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 99.Chen T-W, Wardill TJ, Sun Y, Pulver SR, Renninger SL, Baohan A, Schreiter ER, Kerr RA, Orger MB, Jayaraman V, et al. (2013). Ultrasensitive fluorescent proteins for imaging neuronal activity. Nature 499, 295–300. 10.1038/nature12354. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 100.Batista-Brito R, Vinck M, Ferguson KA, Chang JT, Laubender D, Lur G, Mossner JM, Hernandez VG, Ramakrishnan C, Deisseroth K, et al. (2017). Developmental Dysfunction of VIP Interneurons Impairs Cortical Circuits. Neuron 95, 884–895.e9. 10.1016/j.neuron.2017.07.034. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 101.Ferguson KA, Salameh J, Alba C, Selwyn H, Barnes C, Lohani S, and Cardin JA (2023). VIP interneurons regulate cortical size tuning and visual perception. Cell Rep. 42, 113088. 10.1016/j.celrep.2023.113088. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 102.Friedrich J, Yang W, Soudry D, Mu Y, Ahrens MB, Yuste R, Peterka DS, and Paninski L (2017). Multi-scale approaches for high-speed imaging and analysis of large neural populations. PLoS Comput. Biol. 13, e1005685. 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1005685. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 103.Barson D, Hamodi AS, Shen X, Lur G, Constable RT, Cardin JA, Crair MC, and Higley MJ (2020). Simultaneous mesoscopic and two-photon imaging of neuronal activity in cortical circuits. Nat. Methods 17, 107–113. 10.1038/s41592-019-0625-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 104.Niell CM, and Stryker MP (2008). Highly selective receptive fields in mouse visual cortex. J. Neurosci. 28, 7520–7536. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0623-08.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 105.Mazurek M, Kager M, and Van Hooser SD (2014). Robust quantification of orientation selectivity and direction selectivity. Front. Neural Circ. 8, 92. 10.3389/fncir.2014.00092. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 106.Priebe NJ, Lisberger SG, and Movshon JA (2006). Tuning for spatiotemporal frequency and speed in directionally selective neurons of macaque striate cortex. J. Neurosci. 26, 2941–2950. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3936-05.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 107.Aarts E, Verhage M, Veenvliet JV, Dolan CV, and van der Sluis S (2014). A solution to dependency: using multilevel analysis to accommodate nested data. Nat. Neurosci. 17, 491–496. 10.1038/nn.3648. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 108.Chung Y, Rabe-Hesketh S, and Choi IH (2013). Avoiding zero between-study variance estimates in random-effects meta-analysis. Stat. Med. 32, 4071–4089. 10.1002/sim.5821. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
Data reported in this paper and any additional information required are available from the lead contact upon request.
All original code is available at https://github.com/cardin-higley-lab/CC_TC_Neske_Project.
Any additional information required to reanalyze the data reported in this paper is available from the lead contact upon request.